HEC PRESS Publisher of the
Healthy Eating Club website &
A
sia Pacific Journal of Clinical Nutrition

 


Volume 16 (2007)
1 Issue 1
1 Issue 2
1 Issue 3
1 Issue 4
1 Supplement 1
1 Supplement 2
Volume 15 (2006)
Issue 1
Issue 2
Issue 3
Issue 4
Supplement
Nutrition Society of Australia
Volume 14 (2005)
Issue 1
Supplement on CD
IUNS/APCNS proceedings
Issue 2
Issue 3
Issue 4
Supplement
Nutrition Society of Australia
CURRENT YEAR ISSUES
LOGIN to FULL PAPERS
subscribers only
PAST ISSUES
View full papers (free)
CD-Rom AU$190 vol1-13
NUTRITION SOCIETY OF AUSTRALIA 1976-
View Abstracts
Search our site
 
1000 Asia Pacific J Clin Nutr (1996) 5(3): 196-200

Asia Pacific J Clin Nutr (1996) 5(3): 196-200

Dietary iron intake and iron status of young children

Carol Wham MSc, BHSc, Dip Ed

Paediatric Dietitian, Auckland, New Zealand


Aim. To determine the prevalence of iron deficiency in healthy young children and whether there is an association between food habits and dietary iron intake and iron status.

Methods. 53 children aged 9-24 months were recruited into the study over a 12 month period from a general practice and Plunket child health clinics. Children with intercurrent infections were excluded. Iron status was determined from a full blood count and iron studies. Nutrient intake was assessed by a 24 hour food recall and dietary history questionnaire with nutrient analysis using the New Zealand Food Composition database from the New Zealand Institute of Crop and Food Research Ltd.

Results. 10 children (20%) were anaemic (haemoglobin <110g/L) and 7 children were iron deficient (serum ferritin <10m g/L). The daily mean iron intake was 5.1± 3.1mg, which was 0.66 RDI for 9-12 months, and 0.80 for 12-24 months. There was no statistically significant relationship between iron status and food iron intake. Children in the top quintile for iron intake (mean 17.5 mg/day) consumed iron mainly from iron-fortified formula and baby food whereas the main source of iron in the lowest quintile (mean intake 2.0mg/day) was from a diverse range of foods including vegetables, bread and bakery goods, dairy products, breakfast cereals and fruit. In this group only one child consumed formula and three children consumed baby foods.

Conclusion. A high prevalence of anaemia and of iron deficiency was found amongst the otherwise healthy children in the sample, without their being a relationship between dietary iron intake and either haemoglobin or serum iron indices, except for ferritin.

Key words: Anaemia, children, iron deficiency, iron intake, New Zealand


Introduction

Iron deficiency is a well recognised health problem amongst New Zealand children. Numerous studies over the past thirty years have documented its prevalence particularly amongst infants and young children living in underprivileged circumstances and the children of the Maori and Pacific Islanders1-5. The current prevalence of iron deficiency in otherwise healthy young children is not known.

The major goal in screening children for iron deficiency has been to identify iron deficiency anaemia. However anaemia is only a late manifestation of iron deficiency, and nutritional anaemia is uncommon. The significant consequences of iron deficiency relate to the deficiency in tissue iron, and current interest ce 1000 ntres on the evidence that iron deficiency impairs psychomotor development and cognitive function6,7. These deficits are of particular concern because they may accompany iron deficiency without anaemia, and it is not known whether they are reversible.

There are many factors which contribute to the high prevalence of iron deficiency in children living in underprivileged circumstances: breastfeeding of short duration, lack of availability of iron fortified formula or foods, inadequate or inappropriate weaning foods and lack of medicinal iron for prevention of iron deficiency. In developed countries the availability of dietary iron can be restricted by the early introduction of cow’s milk in infancy and by an excessive cow’s milk intake in young children. Milk not only displaces iron-rich foods from the diet, but the high levels of protein, calcium and phosphorus form insoluble complexes with iron in the intestine8,9. Social and cultural feeding practices such as tea drinking, recognised to be a problem among Pacific Island children, may exacerbate the problem10.

The assessment of food iron intake in relation to iron status has not been included in previous New Zealand studies. A study was therefore planned to assess the prevalence of iron deficiency in healthy young New Zealand children, and to determine its relationship to dietary habits in the weaning period, the type of transitional foods used, and the amount of iron in the diet.

Methods

The subjects comprised 53 healthy children aged between 9 and 24 months living in Auckland. This was an opportunistic sample recruited by a paediatrician and the general practitioners at the Green Bay Medical Centre, West Auckland, and by a community paediatrician at child health (Plunket) clinics around Auckland. Subjects were recruited between August 1992 and August 1993. Criteria for recruitment were attendance for minor non-infectious condition, immunisation, or a well-child check. Children with intercurrent infections were excluded.

The age range of children was evenly distributed between the age groups 9-12, 13-16, 17-20 and 21-24 months. There were slightly more females than males. The standard deviation score (SDS) for weight for males was -0.23, for females 0.08. The mean age of mothers in the sample was 29.4 ± 7.7years, slightly older than the mean age of mothers recorded in the Plunket National Child Health Study (NCHS) of 27.7 years11 and in the December 1990 National Census of 27.2 years12 (Table 2). There were fewer single mothers and Pacific Island and Maori mothers in the sample compared to mothers in the Plunket NCHS (Table 1). Based on the Elley-lrving Socio-Economic Index13 there were fewer fathers in socioeconomic groups 1 and 2, and groups 5 and 6 in the sample compared to fathers in the Plunket NCHS (Table 1).

Parents of eligible children were handed an information leaflet inviting participation in a screening test for iron deficiency in their child. Included was a brief description of the blood test and food intake questionnaire. Those who agreed to take part were advised that the principal investigator would contact them to arrange a home visit to administer the food intake questionnaire after the blood test had been taken. No dietary advice was given at this time.

Blood samples were taken at commercial laboratories. Nurses with experience in venepuncture took 5mL samples from each child. These were analysed at the central Auckland base of Diagnostic Laboratory .

The blood indices measured were serum ferritin, serum iron, total iron-binding capacity, transferrin saturation, haemoglobin (Hb), mean cell volume, mean cell haemoglobin, white blood count, neutrophils and lymphocytes.

Computer analyses of the results were forwarded within 24 hours to the requesting doctor who checked the results for any anomalies and these were not disclosed to the principal investigator until dietary intake data was obtained. Iron deficiency was defined by serum ferritin less than 10m g/L and anaemia was defined by haemoglobin less than 110g/L14-16.

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of study group. 1000
 

Years

n

Sample

Plunketa

Censusb

Maternal

20-24

5

9

21.2

23.2

Age

25-29

15

28

34.7

35.3

 

30-34

14

1000

26

27.9

24.6

 

35-39

16

30

8.6

7.5

 

40-44

3

7

.9

1.0

Family Status (Living arrangements)
Alone with child

2

4

12

na

With partner and child

14

26

 

na

With partner and children

35

66

 

na

Other

2

4

88

na

Socioeconomic status: Elley-lrving socio-economic indexC.

1

5

11

 

7.5

2

5

11

31

11.0

3

17

37

51

23.0

4

12

26

 

33.0

5

6

13

18

17.0

6

1

2

 

8.5

Ethnicity          
Non Maori non Pacific Islander

47

89

82.3

86.9

Maori

5

9

12.0

9.5

Pacific Islander

1

2

5.7

3.6

a. Alison LH 1993; b. New Zealand Government Department of Statistics 1991; c. Elley WB, Irving JC 1985

Table 2. Mean (± SD) Daily Energy and Nutrient Intake by Age Group, Combined Sexes

1000
Age (months)

9-12

13-16

17-20

21-24

 

Mean

SD

Mean

SD

Mean

SD

Mean

SD

Energy (kJ)

3625

650

4190

1259

4325

1531

4549

932

Protein (g)

27.6

8.4

31.6

10.3

38.7

16.3

41

12.2

Fat (g)

32.4

11.0

37.9

14.7

38.1

17.2

44.0

17.2

Carbohydrate (g)

121

37

139.5

59

141.

451

139.8

32.5

Fibre (g)

8.3

5.4

7.8

4.3

7.3

4.8

10.1

5

Vitamin C (mg)

76

55

94

58

72

70

60

36

Iron (mg)

6.2

4.4

5.1

3.4

4.5

3.1

4.8

1000 1.9

Food intake data were obtained by a qualified dietitian unaware of the child’s iron status using the 24-hour recall method and a dietary history questionnaire. Food models, food photographs, and measuring devices were used to help the parent identify serving sizes of food consumed. Information about the child’s milk feeding history and unstructured questioning to determine consumption of iron containing foods was recorded. Nutrient intake was analysed using the New Zealand Food Composition Database (NZ Institute of Crop and Food Research Ltd). The data from the 24-hour food recall was analysed by the programme Diet Cruncher: Diet Analysis software (R. Marshall. Nutricomp. Dunedin,1993). This uses the database from Foodfiles, the New Zealand Food Composition Database from NZ Institute of Crop and Food Research Ltd.

The results were analysed using univariate and multiple regression analysis through the SAS statistical package17.

Results

From the total sample of 53 children tested, the ferritin level was not available for one child nor Hb from three. The median serum ferritin level was 23m g/L, mean 31.67 ± 27.9m g/L. The median Hb was 119.5g/L, mean 116.46 ± 9.67g/L. There were no differences in mean values across the age range of sub-samples at 9-12, 13-16, 17-20 and 21-24 months. Ten children (20%) had a Hb < 110g/L, and 7 had a serum ferritin < 10m g/L.

Breastfeeding was initiated in 94% of the sample, and this continued in 77% of infants at 9 weeks and in 22% at one year. Cow’s milk was introduced at a mean age of 11 months, being the main type of milk drink by 19 months.

The daily mean iron intake was 5.1 ± 3.1mg, which was 0.66 RDI for 9-12 months, and 0.80 for 12-24 months. There was an increase in daily energy and macronutrient intake with age (Table 2).

There was no statistically significant relationship between iron status and food iron intake. Using serum ferritin and Hb as the dependent variables, multiple regression analysis showed no statistically significant relationship between breastfeeding at 3 months, cow’s milk feeding at 9 months, daily protein, vitamin C or iron intake.

The 10 children in the top quintile for dietary iron intake (mean 17.5mg) consumed iron from different types of food compared to children in the lowest quintile (mean 2.0mg). Children taking cow’s milk as a main milk were over-represented in those in the lowest quintile. In the high iron intake group there were only two children aged 11 and 17 months taking cow’s milk as a primary milk drink (50mL and 350mL respectively). In the low iron group there were seven children (mean age 17 months) taking a mean intake of 530mL. Those with the higher iron intake were predominantly formula fed with most of their iron coming from formula and commercial baby food. The 6 major food sources of iron in the study group (which contributed >5% of iron) were baby food, formulae, fast foods (fried fish, chicken and burgers), eggs, meat and breakfast cereals. There was no difference in haemoglobin levels between children with high iron intakes (mean Hb 115g/L) and low iron intakes (mean Hb 118g/L). The mean serum ferritin for children with high iron intakes was higher at 53mcg/L compared to 27mcg/L for children with low iron intakes (p <0.05).

Eleven 1000 percent of mothers gave tea to their children, but the sample size prevented any conclusion being drawn from its potential effect as an inhibitor of iron absorption.

Discussion.

The results of this study show that iron deficiency with or without anaemia is common amongst otherwise healthy children towards the end of infancy and through the second year of life. Although there were marked differences in dietary iron intake according to food choices there was no relationship between dietary intake of iron and iron status.

The validity of results of dietary intake depend on the accuracy of the 24 hour food intake data. Potential errors include both qualitative and quantitative recording errors, memory distortions or under or over-estimation of food items since estimates of portion sizes by food models is not as accurate as weighing or measuring18,19.

These findings are similar to other studies in which no association between nutrient intake and iron status has been demonstrated20-22. The arbitrary cut off points for haematological indices in young children, and the subtle dietary differences over a period of time, cannot be appreciated or quantified over a single day’s observations but the present study combined 24 hour recall and dietary history data for a more representative example of food and nutrient intakes for each child. A longitudinal study would permit analysis of the differences in dietary intake during infancy and weaning and associated iron status.

Macronutrient intakes were similar to previous published studies of children of similar age23. The daily mean iron intake was 0.66 of the RDI for children aged 9-12 months and 0.80 of the RDI for children 12-24 months. This was similar to the daily intake of iron in Swedish children aged 12 months, which was found to be 0.80 of the RDI24. In Australian children aged between 6 months and 3 years the mean daily iron intake was 0.66 of the RDI for children under 12 months from low income families and 0.59 of the RDI for children of the same age from higher income families25. This difference was due to a higher intake of iron rich cereals in children from low income families.

Other studies have shown that infants fed cow’s milk have lower intakes of iron and are more likely to have impaired iron status than formula fed infants, even with the addition of fortified weaning foods26-28. In this study, children who drank cow’s milk had a lower iron intake compared to formula fed children, but there were no differences in iron status between the feeding groups. This may have been due to the small sample size.

Children in the highest quintile for dietary iron intake consumed iron from different types of food compared to children in the lowest quintile. For example, of the children in the high iron quintile there were only two children taking cow’s milk as a main milk drink. They also had a higher vitamin C intake (153mg) compared to those in the low intake quintile (66mg) and their largest contribution of iron intake was from baby food and infant formula (80%). In the low iron intake group there were only three children who consumed baby foods and only one child consumed infant formula.

Establishing strategies for meeting the iron needs of young children requires knowledge of the iron content and the bioavailability of iron in various foods. Although the iron in breast milk is well absorbed, little iron is present. Most of the iron in the infant’s diet consists of iron added in the commercial manufacture of infant foods, especially iron fortified formulas and infant cereals. Independent of gastrointestinal losses, infants fed cow’s milk in the first half year of 1000 life remain at risk of iron depletion even when supplementary iron containing foods are used. Cow’s milk contains low concentrations of iron, and studies in infants show that its absorption is poor29. There is a high concentration of calcium and phosphorus in cow’s milk, more than double the level in breast milk and iron forms an insoluble complex with these minerals.

In conclusion, these results show that although iron deficiency and anaemia are common amongst otherwise healthy young children, and that dietary intake is highly dependent on transitional food choices, the relationship between iron deficiency and a low intake is obscure in this study group. With our current state of knowledge, this points out the importance of testing the effects of intervention or supplementation to determine what public health strategies may be needed to reduce the prevalence of iron deficiency.

Acknowledgment. Thanks are due to the families and children in the study for their willingness to participate. This study was supported financially by an Economics Laboratory Award, granted by the New Zealand Dietetic Association.

This study was undertaken in partial fulfilment of the requirements of Masters of Science at the University of Otago.


Dietary iron intake and iron status of young children

Carol Wham

Asia Pacific Journal of Clinical Nutrition (1996) Volume 5, Number 3: 196-200


References

  1. Prior I. A health survey in a rural Maori community, with particular emphasis on the cardiovascular, nutritional and metabolic findings. NZ Med J 1962; 359: 333-348.
  2. Barry DMJ, Reeve AW. Iron injections and serious gram negative infections in Polynesian newborns. NZ Med J 1973; 78: 376.
  3. Dawson KP, Whimster J. Serum ferritin levels in iron deficient children. NZ Med J 1980; 92: 96-97.
  4. l\/loyes CD, O’Hagan LC, Armstrong C. Anaemia in Maori infants; a persisting problem. NZ Med J 1990; 103: 53.
  5. Crampton P, Farrell A, Tuohy P. Iron deficiency anaemia in infants. 1993 submitted for publication.
  6. Oski FA, Honig AS, Helu B et al. Effect of iron therapy on behaviour performance in non-anaemic, iron deficient infants. Pediatrics 1983; 71: 877-880
  7. Walter T, Kovalskys J, Stekel A. Effect of mild iron deficiency on infant mental development scores. J Pediatr 1983; 102: 519-522.
  8. Hurrell RF, Lynch SR, Trinidad TP, Dassenko SA, Cook JD. Iron absorption in humans as influenced by bovine milk proteins. Am J Clin Nutr 1989; 49: 546-552.
  9. Monsen ER, Cook JD. Food iron absorption in human subjects. the effects of calcium and phosphate salts on the absorption of non-haem iron. Am J Clin Nutr 1976; 29: 1142-1148
  10. Quested C, Hochstein B, Edgar B, Crossley J, Carr P, Hassall I, Elliot R. Relationships between dietary patterns and iron efficiency in South Auckland infants. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society of New Zealand 1980; 5: 116
  11. Alison LH, Counsell AM, Geddis DC. Reasons for early cessation of breast feeding in New Zealand. The Plunke 1000 t national child health study 1990-91. Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology 1993;7:318-33.
  12. Department of Statistics. Fertility. In: Demographic Trends 1990 Wellington, New Zealand 1991; p43.
  13. Elley WB, Irving JC. The Elley-lrving Socio-Economic Index 1981 Census Revision. NZ J Ed Studies 1985; 20: 115-128.
  14. WHO (World Health Organisation). Nutritional anaemia. WHO Technical Report Series No.3. 1972. World Health Organisation, Geneva
  15. Dallman PR, Siimes MA. Percentile curves for haemoglobin and red cell volume in infancy and childhood. J Pediatr 1979; 94: 26-31
  16. Dallman PR, Reeves JD, Driggers DA et al: Diagnosis of iron deficiency: the limitations of laboratory tests in predicting response to iron treatment in 1 year old infants. J Pediatr 1981; 98: 376-381
  17. SAS Institute Inc. Cory, North Carolina.
  18. Sorenson AW, Calkins BM, Connolly MA, Diamond E. Comparison of nutrient intake dertermined by four dietary intake measurements. J Nutr Educ. 1985; 17: 92-99
  19. Livingstone MBE, Prentice AM, Coward WA, Strain JJ, Black AE, Davies PSW et al. Validation of estimates of energy intake by weighed dietary record and diet history in children and adolescents. Am J Clin Nutr 1992; 56: 29-35
  20. Duggan MB, Steel G, Harbottle L, Noble C. Iron status, energy intake and nutritional status of health young Asian children. Arch Dis Child 1991; 66: 1386-1389.
  21. Brault-Dubuc M, Nadeau M, Dickie K. Iron status of French Canadian children: a three year follow-up study. Hum Nutr Appl Nutr 1983; 37A: 210-221.
  22. Arija V, Salas J, Fernandez Ballart J, Marti Henneberg C. Iron deficiency risk in children: discrepancy between dietary and biochemical assessments. Int J Vitam Nutr Res 1990; 60: 150-155.
  23. Boulton TJC, Coote LM. Nutritional studies during childhood. I. Energy and nutrient intake. Aust Paediatr J 1979; 15: 72-80.
  24. Persson LA, Johansson E, Samuelson G. Dietary intake of weaned infants in a Swedish community. Hum Nutr: Appl Nutr 1984; 38: 247-254.
  25. Lovric VA, Lammi AT, Friend JCM. Nutrition, iron intake and haematological status in healthy young children. Med J Aust 1972; 1: 11-14.
  26. Pizzaro R, Yip R, Dallman PR et al. Iron status with different infant feeding regimes: relevance to screening and prevention of iron deficiency. J Pediatr 1991; 118(5): 687-692.
  27. Martinez GA, Ryan AS, Malec DJ. Nutrient intakes of American infants and children fed cow’s milk or infant formula. Am J Dis Child 1985; 139: 1010-1018.
  28. Mills AF. Surveillance for anaemia: risk factors in patterns of milk intake. Arch Dis in Child 1990; 65: 428-431.
  29. Lonnerdal B. Cows’ milk during the second half-year of life; effect on iron status. Editorial. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 1990; 11(3): 302-303.


Copyright © 1996 [Asia Pacific Journal of Clinical Nutrition]. All rights reserved.
Revised: January 19, 1999 .
to the top

0